I am maintaining the following paper here just in case I lose contact
with where it is maintained. Mike
The Relationship Between Schizophrenia & Mysticism:
A Bibliographic Essay
6/92, Sandra Stahlman
email me at [email protected]
The nature of schizophrenia, and other forms of psychosis, is still under
debate and a significant issue is the relationship between psychosis and
the mystical, or religious, experience. Throughout history this question
has been addressed by scholars from all fields of inquiry. Currently,
psychologists are looking at the similarities and differences between the
experiences, hoping to shed light on the nature, process, and treatment of
psychosis. I was curious to see what has been discovered.
The information available on mystical experience and psychotic episodes seems
limitless. This paper will focus on the current psychological perspective
which examines mystical and psychotic experiences as a natural, universal
phenomena. This is not a new idea; however, specific to recent research is
its objective, systematic nature. Looking to define both in value-neutral,
experiential terms, psychologists are scrutinizing the biological, psychological,
and behavioral correlates of the two experiences, combining information from
ongoing measurement and personal interviews and the body of knowledge available
from philosophy and the study of religion.
In order to discover the relationship between mystical experience and psychosis,
analysis must first be directed at defining each individually. Therefore
I present not only a review of the current psychological research regarding
this relationship, but also a look at theories addressing the questions "what
is a mystical experience?" and "what is psychosis?"
DEFINING MYSTICISM
One topic scholars agree on is the need for a formal definition of mystical
experience; they intend to determine the characteristics common to all
descriptions of mystical experience, and have this be a "working definition"
for present research. In this manner, studies can be related simply and without
confusion of terms. Authors of the material I reviewed began discussion with
their definition of mystical experience; the following characteristics emerged
repeatedly as each author attempts to define mystical experience: experience
of unity, intense affective experience, time/space distortion, noetic quality,
ineffability, and a sense of holiness or sacredness. In addition, the authors
generally included the concept of universality in their definitions. There
are aspects of the definition which scholars are not able to agree on; however,
the recent development of tools of measurement (eg scales, questionnaires)
may be able to provide information to help answer such questions.
During the early 1900's, William James wrote about the idea of a spectrum
- or continuum - of mystical states of consciousness ranging from the
non-religious to the most religiously profound (James, 1985). Beginning with
the "simplest" sort of mystical experience, James notes the strong sense
of significance and knowledge associated with the experience, its "noetic"
quality. It is one of four qualities that James uses to define mystical states
of consciousness. "Ineffable" is another characteristic which marks an experience
as mystical; the experience defies expression. Due to its subjective nature,
the experience is much like states of feeling. James asserts that these two
qualities "entitle any state to be called mystical" (p.302). However, there
are other qualities usually associated with the experience. He explains that
the experiences are generally transient. Fading quickly, it is hard to recall
the quality of the experience in memory; they remain just out of reach. But,
some memory content always remains, and this can be used to "modify the inner
life of the subject between the time of their recurrence" (p.303). When having
a mystical experience, however, individuals do not seem to actively process
the information. Instead it is a passive experience - James' fourth
characteristic mark. Even though people actively study and/or practice techniques
to produce mystical states of consciousness, once occurring, the experience
seems to happen without their will.
Later, James goes on to suggest that these experiences occur as our "field
of consciousness" increases (James, 1980). One can assert these "simple"
experiences connote a slight widening of this field, whereas the more profound
experiences come when consciousness expands to include items usually filtered,
hidden, or just out of reach. Such could include memories and sensations.
As awareness increases to include more external and internal information,
a sense of self, a boundary between self and environment, expands, seems
to dissipate. The experience is one of unity with information formerly defined
as non-self. This expansion of the self, often referred to as loss of self,
may not be beneficial for someone who does not have a "strong" sense of self
to begin with. To these people, a mystical experience can be frightening
and confusing, to say the least.
In his earlier writings, James refers to "diabolic" mysticism (p.337). Half
of mysticism, he explains, is not a religious mysticism, but cases where
"mystical ideas" are seen as symptoms of insanity. He refers to these as
"lower mysticisms," springing forth from the same psychological mechanisms
as the classic, religious sort. However, the messages and emotions are
experienced as negative. This idea does not combine well with his proposed
spectrum of mystical states of consciousness, where simple experiences are
also referred to as non-religious, but are not accompanied by negative affect.
James reconciles the difference, and concludes that the definition of mystical
states must be value-neutral. All mystical experience, he writes, whether
experienced as positive or negative, deserves recognition as available states
of consciousness. He ends debate over which is a superior form of consciousness;
instead he suggests that, like our rational states, mystical states encompass
both truth and deception, pleasure and pain.
In the essay "Religious Aspects of Peak-Experiences" (1970), Abraham Maslow
uses the term "peak-experiences" to encompass the spectrum of mystical states
of consciousness. He wishes to secularize the experience because he feels
the phrase "mystical" has taken on purely religious connotations. To define
peak-experiences, Maslow presents a list of characteristics which encompass
all varieties of peak-experience. He describes how the experience tends to
be unifying, noetic, ego-transcending; it gives a sense of purpose to the
individual, a sense of integration.
Addressing the concepts "unifying" and "ego-transcending" seems vital because
the sorts of phrases turn up again and again in literature on mysticism.
F.C. Happold (1975) writes "unless the idea of non-duality can be grasped
the range of mystical experience is incomprehensible" (p.71). "Duality" describes
the manner in which we usually perceive our self in relation to the environment.
A division of "self" and "other" occurs. "Ego" can be used to refer to that
self which we are aware of. What happens during a mystical experience has
been described as transcending this ego, or going through a process of temporary
"ego-loss." As multiplicity ceases, the experience is of a mode of consciousness
often referred to as "the One."
The notion of the One is integrated into many cultures; religious traditions
and ceremonies often focus on the culmination of this experience of
transcendence. As the experience closes, it is subsequently interpreted by
the individual's personal ideology. It follows that if this ideology is
religious, the experience will be interpreted as such. Conversely, if the
individual does not hold any religious ideology, the experience will be
interpreted with non-religious connotations. But, why did the experience
come under a religious framework in the first place. What about the mystical
experience is religious? Andrew Greeley (1974) expresses with great emotion
that the underlying message of the mystics' accounts is that "love" is at
the core of the universe (p.79). Accompanying the mystical experience is
often extreme joy, or exultation. Mystics often describe feeling so wonderful,
that they later conclude it was the working of a higher force. Sometimes
the experience is so emotionally overwhelming that it completely alters the
individual's lifestyle.
However, throughout history, there are accounts of mystical experiences which
are accompanied by strong negative emotions - the diabolic mysticism James
wrote of. Later James concludes that both positive and negative forces must
exist in the mystical realm. These negative forces were commonly assumed
to be demons, or the devil. This is how they were interpreted. Could it be
then, that insanity is the current interpretation of "negative" mystical
experiences? It would be difficult for a mystic to integrate into a society
which held no such role. Are some of the insane actually mystics? As I will
describe in detail later, there are now diagnostic questionnaires which doctors
can use to determine if an experience is, by definition, mystical.
Kenneth Wapnick (1980) explains that mystics tend to follow a very structured,
common process, culminating with the mystical experience. He refers to an
outline of this process created by Underhill in 1961, in which the mystic
moves from "an awakening of self" (p.323) to the purgation of attachments
to the social world and the self, resulting in an experience of "a state
of pure consciousness, in which the individual experiences nothing" (p.324).
Wapnick has added a final "step" to Underhill's outline; most mystics happily
and successfully reintegrate into the world of social attachments. Wapnick
points out that it is attachment to the social world that trained mystics
renounce through their process, not the social world itself. Many individuals
devote their lives to cultivating a mystical experience; methods of meditation,
ritual, and dance - for example - are used to induce transcendence.
For other individuals, the experience occurs spontaneously, in seemingly
any situation, with religious or non-religious connotations. Andrew Greeley,
a priest and author, is one of many scholars who have conducted a "census"
survey to determine what portion of the US population report having had a
mystical experience. His preliminary finding show that a substantial percent
have had the experience. He notes that they range from mild to intense, rare
to frequent. Greeley defines the experience as "something like Maslow's
peak-experience, that is, a feeling of intense unity with the universe and
of one's place within that unity" (p.12). He stresses that mystics describe
the experience as more of an experience of cognition than of feeling; the
mystic comes to know something previously unknown.
Raymond Prince (1979) discusses four other population surveys regarding mystical
experience. All four discovered that 20-40% of those surveyed report a mystical
or religious experience. Some respondents did not know the concept of mystical
experience, yet fulfilled requirements. Findings show that the experience
is more common when the individual is in good mental health. Scholars maintain
that these percentages, along with cross-cultural evidence, warrant considering
mystical experience a universally occurring natural phenomenon.
Universality of the mystical experience is addressed Robert Ornstein (1977).
He explains that all individuals have access to knowledge beyond the intellectual
sort, knowledge that is often ignored in our culture. Ornstein supplies many
example of this knowledge - creative wisdom and insight from dreams, body
temperature patterns, chemical reactions on a cellular level, and he postulates
there are many forms of information we are as yet oblivious to. He asserts
that we are equipped with the "tools" to access both the realms of rationality
and intuition. Ornstein presents a chronicle of the definition of consciousness
throughout history. He stresses the dialectic of theories, which tend to
describe two polar facets of consciousness, the rational and intuitive modes
of operation. He suggests we follow a lesson found cross-culturally: the
most effective mode of operation appears to be one that synthesizes the two
ways of engaging the world.
Deikman's essays (1980) describe the process of "deautomatization," in which
"active" information-limiting processes which filter and analyze give way
to a "receptive" mode. Deikman refers to this process as "perceptual expansion;"
awareness includes stimuli which are usually filtered or repressed, such
as our own electrochemical processes. As such information-limiting processes
are deautomatized, boundaries of self expand to include a wide source of
knowledge previously withheld from conscious awareness - information we (our
unconscious processes) would "normally" filter from awareness. In the receptive
mode we are able to see the greater picture, as systems to discriminate and
categorize are reduced to a minimum. We are able to reconcile difference,
as paradox is tolerable. We are open to creative knowledge. Deikman applies
this principal to the unusual sensations which accompany the mystical experience.
In such instances, he stresses, it is the mode of perception which has changed,
not the external stimuli themselves. Consider the experience of time.
Perceptually, a minute can seem fleeting, or appear to drag incessantly;
but, "normal" clock time would be identical in both cases.
Robert Ornstein's discussion of "reality" and what is considered "normal"
consciousness raises an important issue, especially as it relates to mental
health. He explains that normal reality is a consciousness which can be shown
(through sensory experimentation, for example) to be a constructed reality;
in order to create a stable, manageable environment, a sensory-filtering
system develops from childhood and continually shaped by subsequent situations.
What is experienced as reality, Ornstein explains, is actually only a
representation. If "normal" consciousness is created, he concludes, then
this consciousness may be altered simply by changing the manner of it
construction. The mystical experience can be seen as a transcendence of normal
consciousness and reintegration; the knowledge gained from the experience
offers beneficial growth-potential.
Ornstein's point-of-view exemplifies the style, or manner, in which the subject
of mysticism has come to be studied. By describing the experience using neutral,
bio-psychological terms, scholars such as Ornstein and the others I have
referred to enable mystical experience to be studied as a universal - and
not necessarily religious - experience of consciousness. This is essential
to an accurate examination of the similarities and differences between mystical
experience and psychosis.
DEFINING PSYCHOSIS
David Lukoff (1985) speaks of the criteria which must be met to acquire the
label "psychotic." He writes:
"The phenomenology (imagery, cognitions) of the psychotic condition shares
many characteristics with dream experiences (Hall,1977), hallucinogenic drug
trips (Kleinman et al,1977), spiritual awakenings (Assigioli, 1981), near
death experiences (Grof & Grof,1980) and shamanic experiences (Halifax,
1979). The fantastic or bizarre content of reported experiences is not sufficient
indication that a person is psychotic" (p.162).
What does indicate psychosis? Lukoff explains that doctors must decide whether
or not the patient's cognitions are "understandable." Psychotics are individuals
whose inner workings are not comprehensible. In addition, doctors look to
see if the patient is able to function in everyday life; do they possess
common sense? "Psychosis is considered a disruption to the normal functioning
of consciousness," explains Lukoff.
R.D. Laing (1967) criticizes this method of diagnosis. In his book, The Politics
of Experience, Laing points out that the decision is wholly subjective on
the part of the doctor. He fears that physicians do not actively attempt
to understand patients' communications, and meanings and connections are
missed - although they do exist from the perspective of the patient. Laing
supports the opinion that you cannot possibly understand the inner mind of
an individual if you do not know his or her background.
A college-level textbook, Abnormal Psychology: Current Perspectives, defines
psychosis as a class of psychological disorders in which "reality contact"
is "radically impaired" (p.348). The authors explain reality contact as the
capacity to perceive, process and respond to stimuli in an adaptive manner.
The text explains that psychoses can be classified as biogenic or functional.
The biogenic psychoses are those associated with known physical causes. With
functional psychoses, on the other hand, no physical cause can be clearly
identified; thus, the basis of the psychoses may be "at least partly psychogenic"
(p.348).
The functional psychoses are divided into three categories: mood disorders,
schizophrenia, and the delusional disorders. Mood disturbances are disorders
of affect; schizophrenic and delusional disorders are considered to be disorders
of thought (p.384). Disorders of affect and thought are explained in greater
detail in S. Epstein's article "Natural Healing Processes of the Mind: Acute
Schizophrenic Disorganization" (1979). Epstein describes two common symptoms
of impaired cognitive functioning: a loss of integrative capacity and lower
perceptual systems. With the loss of integrative capacity, the ability to
make inferences about size, distance, depth, and other relational cues break
down. Perceptual sensations become distorted as the performance of the lower
order functions becomes impaired. Epstein explains that "when there is a
partial breakdown of cortical control, the individual tends to experience
current situations with a sharpened intensity" (p.318). Such heightened intensity
of perceptions can be explained by the lack of organizational capacity. Epstein
notes that:
"This, together with a release from inhibition of repressed memories and
impulses, confronts the individual with the raw data of new experiences and
with unassimilated old experiences that can no longer be ignored, and that
can now be experienced uninfluenced by the biasing lenses of the old conceptual
system [ego]. The weakened inhibitory control may also foster abreaction.
The overall process provides an unusual opportunity for new learning and
the assimilation of old learning to occur" (p.318).
However, Epstein cautions, whereas it is evident that acute schizophrenic
disorganization can be beneficial, this growth potential is not characteristic
of the whole range of psychosis; it is usually limited to psychotic episodes
which come on quickly, are precipitated by a stress-inducing event, and last
only for a short time ("acute"). The level of everyday functioning before
psychosis is also a good indication of the prognosis.
THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN MYSTICISM AND PSYCHOSIS
What does the research I reviewed say about the relationship between mystical
experience and psychotic episodes? The majority of scholars come to conclude
that mystical experience and psychosis are both examples of naturally available,
altered states of consciousness. Investigations explore the "location" of
these experiences within the range of altered states of consciousness. Efforts
are being concentrated on designing measurements systems to determine the
characteristics which distinguish the two experiences.
Peter Buckley (1981) presents findings from an examination of reports of
mystical experiences and episodes of acute psychosis. The purpose of his
study is to see what is the relationship between the two experiences, as
there are many evident similarities. Buckley writes,
"The appearance of a powerful sense of noesis, heightening of perception,
feelings of 'communion' with the 'divine', and the exultation may be common
to both. The disruption of thought seen in acute psychosis is not a component
of the accounts of mystical experience reviewed by the author, and auditory
hallucinations are less common than visual hallucinations in the mystical
experience" (p.516).
Buckley goes on to suggest that the two experiences are both elements of
a "limited repertoire of response within the nervous system for an altered
state experience" (p.516) -- similar, but distinct elements.
Delving further into the relationship, Buckley notes that one subtype of
schizophrenia is less distinct from mystical experience. He explains that
Schizophreniform Psychosis episodes are generally without auditory
hallucinations, delusions, or impaired social relations. In addition, the
episodes are generally brief, like the mystical experience. Other forms of
schizophrenia, in contrast, may last for great lengths of time. Buckley explains
that schizophreniform psychoses are believed to be "a variant of the affective
disorders" (p.520). He suggests that what acute psychosis and mystical experience
share "is simply and ecstatic affective change which imbues perception with
an increased intensity" (p.520). Note that the term "acute" describe brief
psychotic episodes with a very short onset.
David Lukoff defends a similar position in his article "" when he criticizes
the DSM-III-R, a diagnostic system used nationwide by psychiatrists. Specifically
he proposes a change in the classification of psychotic episodes; he has
designed - within the system's guidelines - a new diagnostic category. MEPF
for short, a Mystical Experience With Psychotic Features would be a distinct
category within the DSM-III-R. Lukoff argues that the current version of
the DSM does not distinguish psychotic episodes which have a positive outcome.
Lukoff explains that these episodes are brief, come on quickly (acute), and
often result in "improvements in the individual's functioning" (p.157); Lukoff
believes they should be categorized and treated differently from psychotic
episodes which "indicate mental disorder" (p.157).
Lukoff proceeds under the assumption that there are mystical experiences,
psychotic episodes, mystical experiences with psychotic features, and psychotic
disorders with mystical features. Lukoff explains that MEPF would describe
"the presence of the psychotic state during an essentially religious experience"
(p.166). Three criteria for the MEPF are: an overlap with mystical experience,
positive outcome likely, and low-risk - an "exclusionary criterion" to be
implemented "only if the danger seems immediate and severe" (p.171). Five
categories define the overlap with mystical experience: ecstatic mood, sense
of newly-gained knowledge, perceptual alterations, delusions (if present)
have themes related to mythology, and finally, no conceptual disorganization
is apparent. In addition, two of the following must be present to fulfill
the positive outcome likely criteria: good pre-episode functioning, acute
onset of symptoms during 3 months or less, a stressful precipitant to the
episode, and a positive attitude towards the experience. If these criteria
are met, MEPF would be the diagnostic category to use. It would be assumed
that the experience will be relatively brief, and beneficial (assist growth).
The treatment would differ from the other psychoses, where persistent conceptual
disorganization and successful reintegration into society is unlikely.
Lukoff presents an example case study. In addition, he supplies references
to scales and tests which may be used to make a diagnosis of MEPF. For example,
he suggests a scale and interview created by N. Andreasen (1979) which can
help determine if conceptual disorganization is present; because, Lukoff
notes, that bizarre speech does not always indicate disorganization.
Questionnaires and scales can yield a wealth of information if administered
in a precise manner. Hood and Morris (1981) created the Mystical Experience
Questionnaire. It was a culmination of "the major component criteria common
in empirical studies of mysticism" (p.77). The questionnaire items were derived
from the "research of Brown, Spilka, and Cassidy (1978), Greeley (1974),
and Hood (1975)" (p.77). Examining these sources, I found that they all were
in turn, based upon the research of Stace. In 1960, Stace created categories
to distinguish the "core" mystical experience. First Stace differentiates
between "introvertive" and "extrovertive" mystical experience, the extrovertive
being "on a lower level than the introvertive type...a partly realized tendency
to unity which the introvertive kind completely realizes" (Stace,1960,p.132).
Next, examining the reports of mystics, he generates two lists of common
"core" characteristics, one for each type - introvertive or extrovertive.
Characteristics include: unity, noesis, disregard of logic, bliss, sacredness,
paradox, and ineffability. The Hood and Morris 1981 questionnaire is based
largely upon Stace's conceptualizations.
Michael Siglag administered the Hood and Morris questionnaire to seventy-five
schizophrenic adult inpatients. He describes the research in "Schizophrenic
and Mystical Experiences: Similarities and Differences" (Siglag, 1987).
Participants were chosen from a cross-section of socioeconomic status, ethnic
groups and religious orientations (p.2). Among their initial hypothesis was
a prediction that one-third of the questioned schizophrenics would respond
positively to having had a mystical experience; in addition, those who respond
positively will score equal to or greater than "schizophrenic subjects who
do not claim mystical experience, on the questionnaire's factors measure
experience of unity, affect, time/space distortion, and noesis.
Siglag tells us that 52% of the schizophrenic respondents reported having
a mystical experience, "supporting the idea that the schizophrenic population
perceive themselves as having mystical experience at least as often as
individuals in nonschizophrenic populations" (p.4). In addition they scored
significantly above those schizophrenic that did not report a mystical experience
as hypothesized. Data analysis lead Siglag to the following conclusions:
"Schizophrenic individuals who claim to have had a mystical experience are
similar to other schizophrenic individuals in that they:
1. do not feel any greater control over their experiences than other
schizophrenics;
2. do not experience a greater since of coping ability than other
schizophrenics;
3. do not experience any more improvement in their relationships than other
schizophrenics;
4. experience terror, fear, depression, and a sense of insecurity.
Schizophrenic individuals who claim to have had a mystical experience differ
from other schizophrenic individuals in that they:
1. are more likely to have experienced a sense of unity, oneness, or
connectedness in the world;
2. report more of a range of affective experiences, and are more likely to
have experienced joyful, peaceful states of consciousness;
3. are more likely to report time-space distortions;
4. experience more of a sense of sacredness or holiness;
5. are more likely to see their experiences as valid and meaningful than
other schizophrenics" (pp.10-11).
Siglag explains the implications of such findings; if it could be determined
which patients were involved in the mystical process as well as the psychosis,
therapy could be directed at integration of the "knowledge" acquired - utilizing
the growth-potential of the mystical experience.
Bibliography
Andreasen, N. "The Clinical Assessment of Thought, Language and Conceptual
Disorders." Archives of General Psychiatry, 36; 1979: 1325-1330.
Bootzin, Richard R. and John Ross Acocella. Abnormal Psychology: Current
Perspectives (5th edition). Random House: New York, 1988.
Brown, G.A, B. Spilka, and S. Cassidy. "The Structure of Mystical Experience
and Pre- and Post Experience Lifestyle correlates." Presented at the Convention
for the Scientific Study of Religion. Hartford, CT. October 7, 1978.
Buckley, Peter. "Mystical Experience and Schizophrenia." Schizophrenia Bulletin,
7; 1981: 516-521.
Deikman, A. "Deautomatization and the Mystic Experience." Understanding
Mysticism. Image Books: Garden City, 1980
Deikman, A. "Bimodal Consciousness and the Mystic Experience." Understanding
Mysticism. Image Books: Garden City, 1980.
Epstein, A. "Natural Healing Processes of the Mind: I. Acute Schizophrenic
Disorganization." Schizophrenia Bulletin, 5; 1979: 313-320.
Greeley, Andrew M. Ecstasy A Way of Knowing. A Spectrum Book: Englewood Cliffs,
1974
Happold, F.C. Mysticism: A Study and Anthology. Penguin Books: Harmondsworth,
1975.
Hood, Jr, Ralph W. "The Construction and Preliminary Validation of a Measure
of Reported Mystical Experience. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,
14; 1975: 29-41.
Hood, Jr, Ralph W. and Ronald J. Morris. "Knowledge and Experience Criteria
in the Report of Mystical Experience." Review of Religious Research, 23;
1981: 76-85.
James, William. The Varieties of Religious Experience. Harvard University
Press: Cambridge, 1985.
James, William. "A Suggestion about Mysticism." Understanding Mysticism.
Image Books: Garden City, 1980.
Laing, R.D. The Politics of Experience. Pantheon Books: New York, 1967.
Lukoff, David. "The Diagnosis of Mystical Experience With Psychotic Features."
Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 17; 1985: 155-181.
Maslow, Abraham. "Religious Aspects of peak-experiences." Personality and
Religion. Harper & Row: New York, 1970.
Ornstein, Robert E. The Psychology of Consciousness. Harcourt Brace Joavonovich,
Inc.: New York, 1977.
Prince, Raymond. "Religious Experience and Psychosis." Journal of Altered
States of Consciousness, 5; 1979: 167-181.
Siglag, Michael A. "Schizophrenic and Mystical Experiences: Similarities
and Differences." Presented at the 95th Annual Convention of the American
Psychological Association. New York, NY. August 30, 1987.
Stace, W.T. Mysticism and Philosophy. J.B. Lippincott Company: Philadelphia,
1960.
Wapnick, Kenneth. "Mysticism and Schizophrenia." Understanding Mysticism.
Image Books: Garden City, 1980.